(Photo credit: © Bazruh | Dreamstime.com)
FREIBURG, Germany — Memory shapes behavior across the animal kingdom, from elephants recognizing long-lost herd members to dogs remembering where they buried their favorite toys. Now, research reveals that even tiny ant brains can form lasting memories of hostile encounters, leading them to hold what amounts to insect grudges against rival colonies.
Scientists at the University of Freiburg have discovered that ants possess a sophisticated learning ability that helps them recognize and respond more aggressively to ants from colonies that previously attacked them. This study, published in Current Biology, proves even neighborhood feuds are occurring in tiny colonies underground.
The study focused on the black garden ant (Lasius niger), a common species found throughout Europe. These ants live in colonies that frequently interact with neighboring groups as they forage for food and defend their territory. Each nest maintains its own unique chemical signature or scent, which ants use to distinguish friends from foes. When encountering rivals, they may open their mandibles threateningly, bite aggressively, or even spray acid to kill their competitors.
In a series of elegant experiments, the research team exposed individual ants to brief encounters with ants from other colonies. These meetings lasted just one minute each day over five days. The scientists carefully tracked the ants’ aggressive behaviors, such as opening their mandibles threateningly or attempting to bite their opponents.
As it turns out, the ants became increasingly aggressive toward individuals from colonies they had previously fought with, while showing less hostility toward unfamiliar ants from colonies they had never encountered. This pattern suggests that ants can form specific memories of their adversaries and adjust their behavior accordingly.
Even three brief encounters within a 45-minute period were sufficient for ants to develop enhanced recognition of their opponents, though the effect was stronger when the learning occurred over several days.
Most intriguingly, the research revealed what scientists call a “nasty neighbor effect.” Ants showed the highest levels of aggression toward colonies located within their foraging range (about 5 meters), compared to colonies situated farther away. This heightened hostility toward nearby rivals makes evolutionary sense, as neighboring colonies compete directly for resources and represent the most immediate threat.
The learning process appears to be driven by associative learning, similar to how Pavlov’s dogs learned to associate a bell with food. In this case, ants associate the chemical signatures of rival colonies with the aggressive encounters they experience. When researchers prevented aggressive interactions by disabling the attacking abilities of some ants, the learning effect disappeared, confirming that the physical confrontation serves as a crucial trigger for memory formation.
This mechanism helps explain why older worker ants, particularly foragers who spend more time outside the nest, tend to be more aggressive than their younger nestmates. Their increased exposure to rival colonies provides more opportunities to learn and remember potential threats, making them more effective colony defenders.
“We often have the idea that insects function like pre-programmed robots,” says Dr. Volker Nehring, research associate in the Evolutionary Biology and Animal Ecology group at the University of Freiburg, in a statement. “Our study provides new evidence that, on the contrary, ants also learn from their experiences and can hold a grudge.”
Looking ahead, the research team plans to investigate whether and to what extent ants adapt their olfactory receptors based on these experiences, potentially uncovering the physiological basis for this remarkable learning ability.
In the end, it seems these tiny insects have more in common with elephants than we realized – they too never forget, especially when it comes to their enemies.
Paper Summary
Methodology
The researchers conducted four main experiments. First, they studied how ant aggression varied with distance between colonies in natural settings. They then tested how ants responded to repeated encounters with specific rival colonies, both over several days and in rapid succession. Finally, they investigated whether the aggressive interaction itself was necessary for memory formation by using ants with disabled attacking abilities. Each experiment used careful controls and standardized measures of aggressive behavior.
Results
The study found that ants consistently showed higher aggression toward colonies they had previously fought with, compared to unfamiliar colonies. Neighboring colonies (within 5 meters) triggered the strongest aggressive responses. The learning effect occurred both over multiple days and within a single hour, though longer-term exposure produced stronger results. When aggressive interactions were prevented, ants failed to develop enhanced recognition of their opponents.
Limitations
The research primarily focused on one species of ant, and the results might not apply to all social insects. The laboratory conditions, while carefully controlled, may not perfectly replicate natural colony interactions. Additionally, the mechanisms behind the memory formation at the neurological level remain unclear and require further investigation.
Discussion and Takeaways
This research demonstrates that ants can form specific memories of rival colonies based on aggressive encounters, leading to enhanced recognition and increased aggression in future interactions. This ability helps explain various observed behaviors in ant colonies, including the nasty neighbor effect and age-related differences in aggression levels among worker ants.
Funding and Disclosures
The study was funded by the German Research Foundation (NE 1969/6-1). The authors declared no competing interests.
Publication Information
Published in Current Biology (Volume 35, Pages 1-6, January 20, 2025), the study was authored by Melanie Bey, Rebecca Endermann, Christina Raudies, Jonas Steinle, and Volker Nehring from the Department of Evolutionary Biology and Ecology at the University of Freiburg, Germany.